Warming Up For Elite Athletes: More Nuanced Than It Seems
Why professional athletes require prolonged warm-ups and how human motor learning involves high noise and plasticity in sensorimotor networks.
HEKA FOR COACHES
Heka Sports
9/30/20255 min read
Paper Discussed: Robert Ajemian , Alessandro D’Ausilio , Helene Moorman & Emilio Bizzi (2010) Why Professional Athletes Need a Prolonged Period of Warm-Up and Other Peculiarities of Human Motor Learning, Journal of Motor Behavior, 42:6, 381-388, DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2010.528262
In competitive sports requiring fine motor skills, professional athletes dedicate significant time to warm-up routines before events. While traditionally attributed to preparing muscles and tissues, recent research suggests deeper neurophysiological processes underlie this necessity. The paper above synthesizes contemporary insights into the peculiarities of human motor learning, focusing on why expert performance demands prolonged warm-up periods and how the sensorimotor system’s intrinsic properties contribute to this phenomenon.
Introduction
Professional athletes performing fine motor skills in sports engage in extended warm-up routines prior to competition. While traditional explanations attribute this to the physical warming of muscles, tendons, and ligaments, this article argues that warm-up serves a more fundamental neurophysiological purpose: recalibrating the athlete’s sensorimotor system.
Despite a lifetime of training yielding stable motor memories, athletes experience a warm-up decrement characterized by reduced initial performance without practice immediately before an event. This decrement has been observed for over a century but remains inadequately explained from a motor neuroscience perspective. Unlike robotic systems that require only minimal warm-up to reach operational readiness, human motor systems need prolonged practice to regain peak performance, indicating that warm-up effects transcend mere physical preparation.
Theoretical Framework
The authors propose a conceptual framework viewing the human motor system as a large, distributed sensorimotor network that learns all motor skills collectively rather than in isolation. At any given time, the network exists in a particular configuration within a high-dimensional weight space that encodes the entirety of learned skills. Learning is conceptualized as movement within this configuration space to reduce motor errors associated with specific tasks.
This distributed architecture inherently faces the Stability–Plasticity dilemma: the system must be plastic enough to incorporate new skills while stable enough to retain previously learned skills. However, because neural resources (weights and nodes) are shared among skills, new learning can interfere with older learned skills, a phenomenon related to catastrophic interference in artificial neural networks.
Unlike many models assuming explicit segregation of neural resources for different tasks, this framework posits no hardwired segregation, making interference a persistent challenge. The system must navigate these competing demands to maintain and update multiple motor memories.
High Noise and High Learning Rate Hypothesis
A key novelty in the framework is the hypothesis that biological sensorimotor networks operate under unusually high noise levels and high synaptic learning rates. Noise reflects intrinsic variability in neural signaling, while a high learning rate implies rapid synaptic changes in response to error signals.
At first glance, these conditions would seem unstable, as noise could overwhelm learning signals. Yet, the authors argue that noise and high learning rate coexist to generate a dynamic balance: synaptic weights continuously fluctuate even when no behavioral error is present, but these fluctuations do not translate into high behavioral variability. Thus, the system behaves as if it has an effective lower learning rate at the behavioral level.
This ongoing synaptic plasticity means that even highly practiced motor skills are subject to subtle destabilization over time, requiring recalibration through warm-up practice to restore optimal network configurations.
Simulation Methodology
To test their claims, the authors employ simulations using modified multilayer perceptrons with significantly elevated learning rates (0.3 vs. typical 0.01) and injected noise at multiple levels (nodal signal processing, weight multiplication, and weight updates). These networks are contrasted with conventional low-noise, low-learning-rate networks.
The simulated task is a center-out reaching movement, a well-studied paradigm in motor control research. The input layer encodes target location, and the output layer produces corresponding motor commands. Learning is driven by gradient descent minimizing mean squared error between desired and actual outputs, with network weights adapting trial-by-trial.
Using this platform, the authors investigate three hallmark phenomena of human motor learning: accelerated relearning (savings), negative transfer, and variability of practice effect.
Accelerated Relearning (Savings)
Phenomenon: After initial adaptation to a perturbation (e.g., visuomotor rotation), relearning the same perturbation occurs faster than initial learning.
Explanation: Within the sensorimotor network framework, learning a skill under perturbation moves the network configuration toward a solution point. Although subsequent practice of different tasks may move the network away, the prior solution point remains closer in weight space than a naive initial configuration. Therefore, relearning is accelerated because the network starts nearer to the solution manifold.
This effect emerges naturally in both high-noise/high-learning-rate and conventional networks, indicating it is a general property of distributed learning systems rather than a marker of noise or learning rate per se.
Negative Transfer
Phenomenon: Practicing a related but distinct skill can impair performance of a previously learned expert skill, despite apparent similarities between the two.
Example: An expert tennis player’s skill may deteriorate if they begin to practice squash, although the tennis expertise confers some advantage in learning squash.
Explanation: Expert skills correspond to network configurations where component skills have become orthogonalized, meaning their neural representations do not interfere. Because tennis and squash strokes share sensorimotor overlap, practicing squash displaces the network from the finely tuned orthogonal configuration of tennis skills, causing negative transfer.
Simulations show that with high noise and learning rates, negative transfer manifests robustly: practicing a similar skill disrupts the original skill’s performance, with the effect size increasing with more practice on the new skill but eventually leveling off. In contrast, conventional networks with low noise and learning rate do not exhibit negative transfer, as expertise forms a stable solution that is immediately recoverable.
Variability of Practice Effect
Phenomenon: Practicing under varied conditions enhances overall learning and retention compared to constant conditions.
Clarification: The effect is more meaningful when the test involves the same task as practiced, rather than a novel task requiring generalization.
Explanation: Conceptually, practicing multiple similar tasks (e.g., reaching with slight perturbations) introduces higher error signals during learning trials, which drives larger weight changes and thus faster progress toward solution points in weight space. This variability prevents premature convergence on suboptimal solutions and promotes exploration.
Simulations demonstrate that networks exposed to variable practice initially learn faster than those practicing a single, fixed task. However, this advantage diminishes over time, with fixed practice eventually surpassing variable practice once near the solution.
Notably, this effect only arises in high-noise, high-learning-rate networks and not in conventional networks, highlighting the importance of these parameters.
Warm-Up Decrement and Sensorimotor Recalibration
The prolonged warm-up period observed in expert athletes is interpreted not as mere physical warm-up but as necessary sensorimotor recalibration. Between practice sessions, synaptic weights in the sensorimotor network continue to fluctuate due to intrinsic noise and plasticity, subtly miscalibrating the system from its expert orthogonalized state.
Thus, warm-up practice serves to restore the network’s configuration, reestablishing the fine-tuned state that supports peak motor performance. The time required for warm-up reflects the extent of miscalibration and the complexity of the skill.
This interpretation predicts that generic physical warm-up without task-specific practice would not substitute for full warm-up in actual skill execution. Indeed, the theory forecasts no performance improvement difference between extensive physical warm-up and minimal warm-up when devoid of actual task practice.
Discussion and Implications
The article advances a parsimonious explanatory framework for several key phenomena in human motor learning grounded in the assumptions of:
High intrinsic noise in biological sensorimotor networks;
High synaptic learning rates facilitating continuous weight fluctuation
This framework accounts for accelerated relearning, negative transfer, variability of practice, and the warm-up decrement as emergent properties of a dynamic sensorimotor network learning multiple overlapping skills in sequence.
The model challenges traditional views that separate motor memory into distinct, stable traces by emphasizing continuous synaptic turnover and distributed representations susceptible to interference unless orthogonalized through extensive practice.
Practical implications extend to training regimens for athletes and rehabilitation strategies, suggesting that skill-specific practice immediately prior to performance is essential to restore sensorimotor calibration. Furthermore, the model encourages reconsideration of training variability and scheduling to optimize orthogonalization and minimize interference.
Conclusion
This article presents a compelling computational and neurophysiological framework showing why professional athletes require prolonged warm-up and highlighting fundamental properties of human motor learning.
It bridges longstanding empirical observations with theoretical models incorporating noise, learning dynamics, and distributed neural representations and is ever-relevant when thinking about skill acquisition and performance optimization.